[1] |
卞迁, 崔磊, 闫国利(2010). 词素位置颠倒对中文句子阅读影响的眼动研究. 心理研究, 3(1), 29-35. |
[2] |
曹海波, 兰泽波, 高峰, 于海涛, 李鹏, 王敬欣(2023). 词素位置概率在中文阅读中的作用: 词汇判断和眼动研究. 心理学报, 55(2), 159-176. |
[3] |
曹洪文(2016). 中文词素位置编码及时空整合的认知研究. 博士学位论文, 成都: 电子科技大学. |
[4] |
丁国盛, 彭聃龄(2006). 中文逆序词识别中整词与词素的关系. 当代语言学, 8(1), 36-45. |
[5] |
顾俊娟, 高志华, 马绍扬(2022). 嵌套词汉字位置加工的亚词边界效应. 心理与行为研究, 20(1), 1-7. |
[6] |
顾俊娟, 高志华, 屈青青(2020). 汉字位置加工的词边界效应. 心理与行为研究, 18(2), 193-199. |
[7] |
顾俊娟, 石金富(2021). 汉字位置加工和词边界效应的认知机制. 心理科学进展, 29(2), 191-201. |
[8] |
蒋理想(2017). 中文阅读中词素与词间位置信息对阅读影响的眼动研究. 硕士学位论文, 福州: 福建师范大学. |
[9] |
彭聃龄, 丁国盛, 王春茂(1999). 中文逆序词的加工——词素在词加工中的作用. 心理学报, (1), 36-46. |
[10] |
吴琼(2013). 语义预测对中文词素位置信息加工的影响. 硕士学位论文, 大连: 辽宁师范大学. |
[11] |
徐迩嘉, 隋雪(2018). 身份信息与位置信息的加工进程及语境预测性的影响. 心理学报, 50(6), 606-621. |
[12] |
Blythe, H. I., Johnson, R. L., Liversedge, S. P., & Rayner, K. (2014). Reading Transposed Text: Effects of Transposed Letter Distance and Consonant-Vowel Status on Eye Movements. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 2424-2440. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-014-0707-2 |
[13] |
Chang, M., Hao, L., Zhao, S., Li, L., Paterson, K. B., & Wang, J. (2020). Flexible Parafoveal Encoding of Character Order Supports Word Predictability Effects in Chinese Reading: Evidence from Eye Movements. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82, 2793-2801. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-020-02050-x |
[14] |
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A Dual Route Cascaded Model of Visual Word Recognition and Reading Aloud..Psychological Review, 108, 204-256. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.108.1.204 |
[15] |
Davis, C. J. (2010). The Spatial Coding Model of Visual Word Identification. Psychological Review, 117, 713-758. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019738 |
[16] |
Davis, C. J., & Bowers, J. S. (2006). Contrasting Five Different Theories of Letter Position Coding: Evidence from Orthographic Similarity Effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 535-557. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.32.3.535 |
[17] |
Grainger, J., & Van Heuven, W. J. (2004). Modeling Letter Position Coding in Printed Word Perception. |
[18] |
Grainger, J., Lété, B., Bertand, D., Dufau, S., & Ziegler, J. C. (2012). Evidence for Multiple Routes in Learning to Read. Cognition, 123, 280-292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.01.003 |
[19] |
Gu, J., & Li, X. (2015). The Effects of Character Transposition within and across Words in Chinese Reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 272-281. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-014-0749-5 |
[20] |
Gu, J., Li, X., & Liversedge, S. P. (2015). Character Order Processing in Chinese Reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41, 127-137. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038639 |
[21] |
Gu, J., Zhou, J., Bao, Y., Liu, J., Perea, M., & Li, X. (2023). The Effect of Transposed-Character Distance in Chinese Reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 49, 464-476. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001180 |
[22] |
Johnson, R. L., Perea, M., & Rayner, K. (2007). Transposed-Letter Effects in Reading: Evidence from Eye Movements and Parafoveal Preview. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 209-229. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.1.209 |
[23] |
Lee, C. E., Pagán, A., Godwin, H. J., & Drieghe, D. (2024). Individual Differences and the Transposed Letter Effect during Reading. PLOS ONE, 19, e0298351. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0298351 |
[24] |
Li, X., & Pollatsek, A. (2020). An Integrated Model of Word Processing and Eye-Movement Control during Chinese Reading. Psychological Review, 127, 1139-1162. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000248 |
[25] |
Li, X., Rayner, K., & Cave, K. R. (2009). On the Segmentation of Chinese Words during Reading. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 525-552. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.02.003 |
[26] |
Liu, D., Chung, K. K. H., Zhang, Y., & Lu, Z. (2014). Sensitivity to the Positional Information of Morphemes Inside Chinese Compound Words and Its Relationship with Word Reading. Reading and Writing, 27, 431-450. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9451-6 |
[27] |
Liu, Z., Li, Y., Cutter, M. G., Paterson, K. B., & Wang, J. (2022). A Transposed-Word Effect across Space and Time: Evidence from Chinese. Cognition, 218, Article ID: 104922. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104922 |
[28] |
Lupker, S. J., & Davis, C. J. (2009). Sandwich Priming: A Method for Overcoming the Limitations of Masked Priming by Reducing Lexical Competitor Effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 618-639. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015278 |
[29] |
Ma, G., Pollatsek, A., Li, Y., & Li, X. (2017). Chinese Readers Can Perceive a Word Even When It’s Composed of Noncontiguous Characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43, 158-166. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000298 |
[30] |
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing Language Profiles in Bilinguals and Multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 940-967. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067) |
[31] |
McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An Interactive Activation Model of Context Effects in Letter Perception: I. an Account of Basic Findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375-407. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295x.88.5.375 |
[32] |
Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., & Tan, L. H. (2005). The Lexical Constituency Model: Some Implications of Research on Chinese for General Theories of Reading. Psychological Review, 112, 43-59. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.112.1.43 |
[33] |
Schoonbaert, S., & Grainger, J. (2004). Letter Position Coding in Printed Word Perception: Effects of Repeated and Transposed Letters. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19, 333-367. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960344000198 |
[34] |
Snell, J., Bertrand, D., & Grainger, J. (2018). Parafoveal Letter-Position Coding in Reading. Memory & Cognition, 46, 589-599. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-017-0786-0 |
[35] |
Sterling, P., & Wickelgren, B. G. (1969). Visual Receptive Fields in the Superior Colliculus of the Cat. Journal of Neurophysiology, 32, 1-15. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1969.32.1.1 |
[36] |
Su, X., Li, X., Li, R., Zhao, C., & Cui, L. (2024). Morphological Structures of Two-Character Words Influence Character Position Encoding. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 56, 383-393. https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2024.00383 |
[37] |
Taft, M., & Zhu, X. (1997). Submorphemic Processing in Reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 761-775. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.23.3.761 |
[38] |
Taft, M., Zhu, X., & Peng, D. (1999). Positional Specificity of Radicals in Chinese Character Recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 498-519. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1998.2625 |
[39] |
Yan, M., Zhou, W., Shu, H., & Kliegl, R. (2012). Lexical and Sublexical Semantic Preview Benefits in Chinese Reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 1069-1075. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026935 |
[40] |
Yang, H., Hino, Y., Chen, J., Yoshihara, M., Nakayama, M., Xue, J. et al. (2020). The Origins of Backward Priming Effects in Logographic Scripts for Four-Character Words. Journal of Memory and Language, 113, Article ID: 104107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2020.104107 |
[41] |
Zhang, L., & Osth, A. F. (2024). Modelling Orthographic Similarity Effects in Recognition Memory Reveals Support for Open Bigram Representations of Letter Coding. Cognitive Psychology, 148, Article ID: 101619. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101619 |
[42] |
Zhang, L., Kang, L., Chen, W., Xie, F., & Warrington, K. L. (2024). Parafoveal Processing of Orthography, Phonology, and Semantics during Chinese Reading: Effects of Foveal Load. Brain Sciences, 14, Article 512. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14050512 |